Published on in Vol 11, No 9 (2022): September

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/36240, first published .
Measuring Microtemporal Processes Underlying Preschoolers’ Screen Use and Behavioral Health: Protocol for the Tots and Tech Study

Measuring Microtemporal Processes Underlying Preschoolers’ Screen Use and Behavioral Health: Protocol for the Tots and Tech Study

Measuring Microtemporal Processes Underlying Preschoolers’ Screen Use and Behavioral Health: Protocol for the Tots and Tech Study

Protocol

Corresponding Author:

Bridget Armstrong, PhD

Department of Exercise Science

University of South Carolina

921 Assembly St.

RM 132

Columbia, SC, 29208

United States

Phone: 1 803 576 8418

Email: ba12@mailbox.sc.edu


Background: Excessive screen time is associated with poor health and behavioral outcomes in children. However, research on screen time use has been hindered by methodological limitations, including retrospective reports of usual screen time and lack of momentary etiologic processes occurring within each day.

Objective: This study is designed to assess the feasibility and utility of a comprehensive multibehavior protocol to measure the digital media use and screen time context among a racially and economically diverse sample of preschoolers and their families. This paper describes the recruitment, data collection, and analytical protocols for the Tots and Tech study.

Methods: The Tots and Tech study is a longitudinal, observational study of 100 dyads: caregivers and their preschool-age children (aged 3-5 years). Both caregivers and children will wear an Axivity AX3 accelerometer (Axivity Ltd) for 30 days to assess their physical activity, sedentary behavior, and sleep. Caregivers will complete ecological momentary assessments (EMAs) for 1 week to measure child behavioral problems, caregiver stress, and child screen time.

Results: The Tots and Tech study was funded in March 2020. This study maintains rolling recruitment, with each dyad on their own assessment schedule, depending on the time of enrollment. Enrollment was scheduled to take place between September 2020 and May 2022. We aim to enroll 100 caregiver-child dyads. The Tots and Tech outcome paper is expected to be published in 2022.

Conclusions: The Tots and Tech study attempts to overcome previous methodological limitations by using objective measures of screen time, physical activity, sedentary behavior, and sleep behaviors with contextual factors measured by EMA. The results will be used to evaluate the feasibility and utility of a comprehensive multibehavior protocol using objective measures of mobile screen time and accelerometry in conjunction with EMA among caregiver-child dyads. Future observational and intervention studies will be able to use this study protocol to better measure screen time and its context.

International Registered Report Identifier (IRRID): DERR1-10.2196/36240

JMIR Res Protoc 2022;11(9):e36240

doi:10.2196/36240

Keywords



Background

Excessive screen time for children is linked with poor sleep, inactivity, and behavioral problems [1-4], and only few children meet the World Health Organization’s recommendation that children under 5 years receive ≤1 hour of screen time per day [5,6]. The ways in which families use screens have changed dramatically over the past 2 decades, in large part owing to the introduction of mobile and interactive digital media devices. Historically, children’s screen time comprised mostly television use, which took place mainly at home. With new forms of digital media such as mobile phones and tablets, families can now use screens across many different locations and contexts. The pervasive availability of mobile devices has resulted in a dramatic increase in children’s use of mobile media. Children’s mobile screen time increased from 5 minutes per day in 2011 to 55 minutes per day in 2020, with nearly half (46%) of the children aged 2 to 4 years and more than two-thirds (67%) of the children aged 5 to 8 years having their own mobile device (tablet or smartphone) [7]. Videos on the web now constitute two-thirds of children’s screen viewing (66%), supplanting traditional television, which now accounts for only 23% of the average daily video screen time [7]. Unfortunately, our understanding of mobile screen use and its impact on child health lags behind the accelerated adoption of mobile technology.

Measuring screen time is complicated and has historically relied upon time-consuming, expensive observational methods conducted by highly trained staff or upon parent recall survey methods [8-12]. In a 2021 systematic review of young (aged 0-6 years) children’s screen time, none of the 622 studies used an objective measure of screen time [13]. However, there was only a moderate correlation between self-reported and objectively observed media use (r=0.38). Of the 49 studies that examined adults’ digital media–use behaviors, only 3 were within 5% of their objectively measured mean [10]. Another important limitation of commonly used screen time measures is their provision of global estimates of screen use, which is often gathered from a single question that asks parents to estimate television use in a typical day [14,15]. This global measure of screen time precludes the ability to examine the context or timing of screen use within and across days, thus limiting the understanding of the complex and changing patterns of children’s daily screen use. Therefore, updated, objective, and low-burden measures of screen use and its context are needed.

Recent developments in the passive monitoring of mobile screen use have the potential to provide novel insights into how, when, where, and why children and families use digital media (ie, mobile phones and tablets) throughout the day [16-18]. This method of digital media monitoring is both less expensive than direct observation and less burdensome for participants than self-reporting measures [15]. Similarly, advances in ecological momentary assessments (EMAs) allow for the assessment of the context and timing of behaviors while minimizing recall bias. Finally, advances in accelerometry have made it possible to passively collect continuous 24-hour data on children’s sedentary behavior, physical activity, and sleep over extended periods (ie, 30 days). While these technologies have been tested in isolation [8,16,19,20], to date, no studies have leveraged multiple streams of data from passive mobile sensing, EMA, and accelerometry to specifically study young children’s screen time [13,21]. It is unclear whether the confluence of these methods can be successfully used to gather meaningful information about the digital media use of children and families. Furthermore, the emerging literature on passive mobile sensing has been conducted with high-income White families and may not be generalizable [8,16].

Objective

We aim to evaluate the feasibility and utility of a comprehensive multibehavior protocol to measure digital media use and screen time context among a racially and economically diverse sample of preschoolers and their families. Figure 1 depicts the conceptual model of the associations between screen time context and children’s behaviors.

Figure 1. Conceptual model of proximal and distal associations among screen use, parent behaviors, and child behaviors.
View this figure

Design Overview

This study includes 100 caregiver-child dyads and uses a longitudinal, observational, dyadic, case-crossover design [22]. Using a case-crossover design allows a dyad to serve as its own control to assess the within-day effects of immediate antecedents on a dependent variable measured multiple times throughout the day and week [22]. Caregivers and their children aged 3 to 5 years will participate in a 30-day assessment protocol, with 50% (50/100) of the sample invited to participate in a second wave of data collection 3 to 12 months later to evaluate the feasibility of retaining the sample over time and to assess the longer-term acceptability of the protocol.

Participants

Participants include racially and economically diverse caregivers and preschoolers in the greater Southeastern United States. The caregiver inclusion criteria are as follows: (1) being a primary caregiver of a child between the ages of 3 and 5 years, (2) owning a smartphone device, and (3) being able to read and speak English. The exclusion criteria for children include a diagnosis of a severe developmental or physical disorder that would prevent ambulation. This decision was made because of the inability to recruit a large enough sample of children to draw meaningful conclusions.

Procedures

The study maintains rolling recruitment, with each dyad on their own assessment schedule, depending on the time of enrollment. Enrollment was scheduled to take place between September 2020 and May 2022.

We aim to recruit a nonrandom volunteer sample by posting fliers at daycare centers, pediatric clinics, and community centers such as food banks, as well as Facebook advertising in the form of “boosted” posts. To obtain a socioeconomically diverse sample, we will partner with daycares serving low-income families and prioritize the enrollment of low-income families. In addition, we will use snowball recruitment methods where participants are compensated for referring families who then successfully participate in the study.

Data collection is completely remote, in part driven by the necessary COVID-19 pandemic protocol adjustments [23]. Interested participants will be directed to an informational website through a QR code or hyperlink. The informational website describes the study procedures and research participant protections. The informational website includes a web-based consent form and directs interested participants to a short screener survey to assess initial study eligibility. A trained member of the research team will contact interested and eligible families by phone to answer any remaining questions and verbally confirm their desire to participate.

Following recruitment, the eligible child and caregiver dyads are texted a Qualtrics link to the baseline survey. The baseline questionnaire is designed to be completed within approximately 30 minutes. After caregivers complete the baseline survey, they are sent instructions to download a screen time monitoring app (Chronicle; Android devices) [18] or upload screenshots (iOS devices) depending on the make of their smartphone. Technical support is provided as required by the research team. Caregivers are then mailed 2 Axivity AX3 accelerometers (Axivity Ltd), one for themselves and one for their preschool-age child. Caregivers and their children are asked to wear the Axivity AX3 and monitor their screen time for 30 days. We will conduct a 30-day assessment to examine the higher limit of time that the families are willing to wear an activity watch and monitor their digital media use. The first week of the 30-day monitoring period includes 7 days of EMAs, which are texted to the primary caregivers’ smartphones. EMAs are limited to the first week (vs the entire monitoring period) to minimize participant burden, as this portion of the study requires active participation. The first week of the 30-day assessment was selected to allow for flexibility because of any potential issues that might prevent a participant from completing EMAs (eg, work conflicts). Participants who do not complete enough EMAs to earn a gift card during the first week (Compensation section) are offered an extension to complete additional EMAs. In line with the recent EMA studies [20], the Tots and Tech study allows flexibility in the survey completion window in an effort to retain families from diverse backgrounds who might experience additional barriers to completing measures within the time frame. Following the 30-day monitoring protocol, caregivers will complete a semistructured qualitative interview about their experiences. Participants who complete the study protocol (eg, do not drop out) in the first year (wave 1) are randomly selected (50/100, 50%) to be recontacted 3 to 12 months after the initial evaluation and invited to repeat the entire research protocol for additional compensation. Families that decline to participate in the second assessment are asked to complete a short web-based survey regarding their experiences and reasons for declining. The number of families that decline or are lost to follow-up is documented to assess protocol feasibility.

Baseline Survey

Overview

The following information is assessed using survey measures administered through the Qualtrics platform. Caregivers report on the following sociodemographic variables for themselves and their child: birthday, biological sex, and race and ethnicity. Caregivers indicate their relationship with the child, marital status, employment status, and education, as well as the following household characteristics: number of children and adults in the home, family income, and use of government assistance (eg, Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program or Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women Infants and Children, medical assistance, etc). Poverty ratio is calculated based on the caregiver’s report of family income and number of dependents using federal poverty thresholds that align with the year of data collection [24]. Caregivers complete the 2-item food insecurity screening questionnaire. This 2-item screener has shown excellent sensitivity and specificity (93% and 87%, respectively) with the 18-item US Department of Agriculture household food security scale [25], which is considered the gold standard for assessing food insecurity among children [26]. A 1-item measure is used to assess neighborhood safety, “How safe do you consider your neighborhood?” [27] Caregivers complete retrospective measures of chronic stress, including perceived stress in the past month (Perceived Stress Scale) [28] and confusion and disorganization in the home environment (CHAOS) [29], in addition to measures of symptoms of depression and anxiety (Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale and State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, respectively) [30,31]. Caregivers also complete measures assessing child behaviors (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire) [32] and parenting satisfaction [33]. Caregivers complete questionnaires regarding their child’s and their own sleep habits and routines [34-36] and screen time habits [37], function [38], and regulations [39], in addition to the number and types of screens in the home and in the child’s bedroom.

Height and Weight

Owing to COVID-19 pandemic restrictions [23], caregiver and child height and weight are collected via caregiver report on the baseline survey. BMI (kg/m2) is calculated for caregivers. As for children, age- and sex-specific BMI z-scores are determined using the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention criteria [40].

EMA Surveys

Caregivers will complete the EMA surveys in the first week of the 30-day protocol. EMA surveys are sent to caregivers via SMS text messages to their smartphones. The SMS text messages contain a Qualtrics survey link to assess the domains using prompts listed in Table 1. Additional items that are only included in the end-of-day survey are listed in Table 2.

Table 1. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) items.
Variable domain and itemResponse optionsFrequency or format
Time spent with child

  • How much time have you spent with your child in the past 2 h?
  • None
  • <30 min
  • 30-60 min
  • 60-90 min
  • 90-120 min

If “none,” skip to Stress Exposure questions
Child mobile phone use

  • In the last 2 h, how much has your child used your mobile phone?
  • None
  • <30 min
  • 30-60 min
  • 60-90 min
  • 90-120 min

Select one
Child other screen time

  • In the last 2 h, how much has your child watched television, played videogames, or used a computer?
  • None
  • <30 min
  • 30-60 min
  • 60-90 min
  • 90-120 min

If “none” on both Child Mobile Phone Use and Child Other Screen Time, skip Screen Function question
Screen function [38]

  • I let my child use these screens...
  • So that they could learn something
  • As a reward for good behavior
  • As they like it
  • As part of a daily routine
  • To allow myself free time

Select all that apply
Child behavior problem intensity [41]

  • In the last 2 h, how problematic has your child’s behavior been?
  • Not at all
  • A little bit
  • Moderate amount
  • Quite a bit
  • A great deal

Select one
Child behavior problem content [42]

  • Did your child lose their temper/have a temper tantrum in the last 2 h?
  • Yes
  • No

Select one

  • Did the tantrum last >5 min?
  • Yes
  • No

If “yes” to the previous question

  • Was this tantrum because they were...
  • Frustrated, angry, or upset
  • Tired hungry or sick
  • To get something she/he wanted
  • Out of the blue
If “yes” to the previous question; select all that apply

  • Did your child disobey or break the rules/say “no” when told to do something in the last 2 h?
  • Yes
  • No

Select one

  • Did your child disobey or break the rules/say “no” because they were...
  • Frustrated, angry, or upset
  • Tired hungry or sick
  • To get something she/he wanted
  • Out of the blue
If “yes” to the previous question; select all that apply

  • Did your child act aggressively in the last 2 h?
  • Yes
  • No
Select one

  • Did your child act aggressively because they were...
  • Frustrated, angry, or upset
  • Tired hungry or sick
  • To get something she/he wanted
  • Out of the blue
If “yes” to the previous question; select all that apply
Stress exposure [43]

  • In the last 2 h, which of these things caused you stress?
  • Work
  • Demands at home
  • Family
  • Tension with a coworker
  • Tension with a partner
  • Tension with your child
  • Something else
Select all that apply
Stress [44]

  • How stressed are you feeling right now?
  • Not at all
  • A little
  • Quite a bit
  • Extremely
Select one
Perceived stress or self-efficacy [45]

  • How certain do you feel that you can deal with all the things that you have to do RIGHT NOW?
  • Not at all
  • A little
  • Quite a bit
  • Extremely
Select one
Affect [46]

  • How frustrated/angry are you feeling?
  • How sad/depressed are you feeling?
  • How happy are you feeling?
  • How calm/relaxed are you feeling?
  • Not at all
  • A little bit
  • Moderate amount
  • Quite a bit
  • Extremely

Select one
Physical context

  • Where were you when you received this message?
  • Home (indoors)
  • Home (outdoors)
  • Work (indoors)
  • Outdoors (not at home)
  • Indoors (not at home)
  • Car/bus/train
  • Other (specify)
Select one
Table 2. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) prompts for end of day.
Variable domain and itemResponse optionsFrequency or format
Parent sleep quality [35]

How would you rate YOUR sleep quality last night?
  • 0 (terrible) to 5 (excellent)
Select one
Child sleep quality [47]

How would you rate your CHILD’S sleep quality last night?
  • 0 (terrible) to 5 (excellent)
Select one
Child behavior

Thinking back on today, how often did your child misbehave in ways that were dangerous or unsafe?
  • Not at all
  • Once
  • More than once
Select one

Thinking back on today, how often did your child act aggressively toward adults?
  • Not at all
  • Once
  • More than once
Select one
Screen time limiting

How much did you stick to your “usual” rules around screen time?
  • 1 (not at all) to 5 (completely)
Select one
Bedtime rules

How much did you stick to your “usual” routines and rules around bedtime?
  • 1 (not at all) to 5 (completely)
Select one
Mobile screen time

For how many hours did your child use your mobile phone today?
  • 0-12+ h

Select one
Interactive screen time

How many hours did your child use a tablet or computer or play video games today?
  • 0-12+ h

Select one
Passive screen time

How many hours did your child watch television, videos, movies (not on a mobile device) today?
  • 0-12+ h

Select one
Time spent with the child [41]

Since waking up this morning, how many hours have you spent with your child in the same location?
  • 0-12+ h

Select one
Daycare

How many hours did your child attend preschool/daycare today?
  • 0-10+ h

Select one
Parent illness

Were you sick today?
  • Yes
  • No
Select one
Child illness

Was your child sick today?
  • Yes
  • No
Select one
Accelerometer compliance-parent

Did you wear the activity watch all day today?
  • Yes
  • No
Select one

If not, why?
  • Lost/cannot locate
  • Strap broken
  • Uncomfortable
  • Other (specify)
If “no” to the previous question
Accelerometer compliance-child

Did your child wear the activity watch today?
  • Yes
  • No
Select one

If not, why?
  • Lost/cannot locate
  • Strap broken
  • Uncomfortable
  • Other (specify)
If “no” to the previous question

Caregivers are informed that they will receive 4 “short surveys” per day between 8:30 AM and 9 PM and that they have 2 hours to complete each survey. For example, if a survey is sent at 8:30 AM, caregivers will have until 10:30 AM to complete the survey. After 2 hours, the links expire, and caregivers will no longer be able to access the survey. The schedule of assessments differs over the course of the 7 days and thus appears random to participants. The exact schedule is presented in Table 3. The timing protocol uses 4 signal-contingent prompts, including 1 end-of-day EMA message, which occurs at 9 PM every night. The EMA surveys are delivered in nonoverlapping time windows and refer to the previous 2 hours. This timing was selected based on previous research indicating that ≤5 prompts per day are acceptable to families [48].

Table 3. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) schedulea.
DayTime
Monday9:30 AM1 PM7 PM9 PM
Tuesday8:30 AM2 PM7:30 PM9 PM
Wednesday9:30 AM2:30 PM6 PM9 PM
Thursday9 AM2 PM7:30 PM9 PM
Friday10:30 AM3:30 PM7 PM9 PM
Saturday9:30 AM3 PM7 PM9 PM
Sunday10 AM2 PM6 PM9 PM

aParticipants will receive an SMS text message at these times prompting them to complete an ecological momentary assessment that must be completed within 2 hours.

In line with the recent guidelines for EMA use [49], caregivers are provided training on completing the EMAs and given a practice opportunity before the start of the assessment period. Training comprises 1 practice EMA and 1 practice end-of-day survey (with additional questions at the day level), and participants have the opportunity to seek clarity on the survey items and procedures before the assessment period.

First, caregivers report how much time they have spent with their child over the previous 2 hours. Caregivers who indicate that they have not been with their child are not presented with survey items regarding their child’s behaviors. EMA prompts regarding the frequency and intensity of child behavioral problems are based on EMA items previously tested among parents of young children [41]. Items regarding specific disruptive behavior are adapted from the Multidimensional Assessment of Preschool Disruptive Behavior [42]. Exposure to stressors is assessed using items adapted from the Daily Hassles Scale [43,50]. Degree of stress is assessed from a single item, “How stressed are you feeling right now?” Responses range from “not at all” to “extremely” [44]. Time Spent with Child and Limit Setting items are informed by existing protocols using EMA among parents [43]. Caregiver report of the overall screen time is assessed using questions adapted from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey screen time survey [51]. Daily assessment of screen time is presumed to improve recall bias compared with 30-day recall measures.

Several methodological considerations were made when designing the EMA protocol to balance the benefits of data richness with the drawbacks of potential participant burden and demand characteristics. Reduced-item EMA subscales are used instead of the full scales in order to limit survey fatigue. In addition, EMA surveys are prompted at seemingly random times within preset intervals (ie, a hybrid signal-interval contingent sampling schedule) to prevent anticipatory effects, such as pausing or changing current behavior in anticipation of a survey prompt at a known time [52]. Despite the use of repeated measures, reactivity is generally low with EMA procedures [53]. Furthermore, the combination of EMA with accelerometer data minimizes the weakness of using either instrument independently [54].

Passive Mobile Sensing (Screen Time Monitoring)

iOS Devices

Caregivers with an iPhone are texted with an automated reminder to send a screenshot of their screen time use each day at 9 PM to a study-specific phone number. The relevant information is located under the “Battery” tab in the iPhone settings. Figure 2 presents an example screenshot. Participants send the research staff 1 practice screenshot (those with iOS devices) before the start of data collection. Trained research staff verify that the iOS screenshot contains the correct information and provide personalized feedback to the participants as needed. If a caregiver fails to send a correct screenshot during the 7 days of EMAs, the study staff send a personalized SMS text message within 24 hours. After the 7-day EMA period, the study staff send a personalized SMS text message if a caregiver fails to send a screenshot on 2 consecutive days (48 h).

Figure 2. Example screenshot to objectively measure screen time on a participants’ iPhone.
View this figure
Android Devices

Caregivers with an Android device are provided instructions to enroll in Chronicle, an app designed specifically for passive screen time monitoring on Android devices [18]. Chronicle collects and transmits the timestamped app use data automatically to its platform. During the 30-day monitoring period, the research staff monitor the Chronicle dashboard daily to ensure regular uploading of the screen time monitoring data. If more than 48 hours has lapsed since the last data uploaded from an active participant, the research staff contact caregivers to troubleshoot and then contact Chronicle support staff as necessary. The use of technical support (participant use of research staff technical support, as well as research staff requests from Chronicle support staff) are recorded as a feasibility outcome.

As caregiver smartphones are monitored, we will use a coding scheme to identify which apps are likely to be used by caregivers versus their children [16]. Two independent coders will categorize each app as either a child or an adult app based on information from the Apple and Google Play app stores; a third coder will arbitrate disagreements. During the qualitative interviews, caregivers are queried about whether their most frequently used apps are primarily used by themselves or their child.

Child Devices

Caregivers whose children have a compatible device (iOS or Android) are invited to enroll their child’s device in the study using the same procedures as those used to enroll parents’ devices. However, child device enrollment is not a requirement for the study.

Accelerometry

Both caregivers and children are asked to wear an Axivity AX3 (Axivity Ltd), a waterproof triaxial accelerometer, on their nondominant wrist for 30 days to assess physical activity, sedentary behavior, and sleep. The nondominant wrist placement improves compliance (compared with waist placement) [55]. Before data collection, the accelerometers are initialized using Open Movement (OMGui, version 1.0.0.43; Newcastle University) at a sample rate of 50 Hz with a range of +8g to −8g. Participants are mailed their accelerometers with reminders about wearing the device, a sticker chart for children to track the number of days worn, and a prestamped envelope to return the accelerometers following the 30-day protocol. The participants are instructed to wear the device at all times, including sleeping periods. In the event of a lost or damaged device, a replacement device is mailed to the participant. The number of lost or damaged devices is used to determine the utility and feasibility of deploying accelerometers over longer wear periods (ie, 30 days).

Data Processing

Axivity data are downloaded using Open Movement (OMGui, version 1.0.0.43; Newcastle University). Raw Axivity AX3 .cwa accelerometer files are processed in R (R Foundation for Statistical Computing) using the GGIR package (version 2.6-4) [56]. During processing, GGIR autocalibrates the signal using local gravity as a reference, identifies abnormally high values, detects nonwear periods, and calculates the magnitude of the acceleration corrected for gravity. Files are excluded if the postcalibration error is >0.02g (a measure of acceleration) and if the wear time is less than 16 hours during the 24-hour period. The nonwear time is determined based on the SD and the value range of each accelerometer axes raw data during 15-minute blocks within a 60-minute window. These blocks are classified as nonwear time if the SD of the 60-minute window is <13 milli-g, and the value range of the 60-minute window is <50 milli-g for 2 of the 3 accelerometer axes [57]. The default method for nonwear time is used, as the average acceleration at similar times on days of the week is imputed for invalid data. We determine the time spent in physical activity intensity categories (eg, inactive, light, and moderate to vigorous) using the intensity thresholds (milli-g) described by Roscoe et al [58] and Hildebrand et al [59,60] for preschoolers and caregivers, respectively. Sleep outcomes are estimated using methods developed by van Hees et al [61], which identify periods of sustained inactivity when the z-angle does not change by >5° for at least 5 minutes.

Qualitative Interview

Following the 30-day monitoring period, caregivers are contacted by phone to complete a semistructured interview with a member of the research team (Multimedia Appendix 1). The semistructured interview is designed to address the following constructs of the study protocol: relevance, comprehensiveness, comprehensibility, satisfaction, and frequency or rationale of nonadherence [62-65]. Feedback provided by caregivers during the semistructured interviews is used to inform protocol changes for the future iterations of the study.

Compensation

In total, caregivers will receive up to US $180 in Amazon gift cards for participating in each wave of the study. Participants will be compensated with electronic gift cards that are delivered after they complete each study task. Caregivers will receive a US $15 gift card for completing the baseline survey. Caregivers who complete at least 21 EMA prompts (75%) will be considered compliant and receive a US $40 gift card. Participants who fail to complete 21 EMA prompts within the 7-day period will be given the option to extend their EMA period beyond 7 days to have additional survey opportunities. Those who complete 21 EMA prompts with additional days of surveys will be compensated the full amount (US $40). Caregivers will receive a US $30 gift card for completing at least 21 days (70%) of screen time monitoring on their smartphones. Caregivers will receive a US $50 gift card upon the return of the Axivity accelerometers; caregiver-child dyads who both wear the Axivity accelerometers for at least 21 valid days (70%) will receive an additional US $25 gift card. Finally, caregivers will be compensated US $20 for completing the semistructured phone interview.

Wave 2

A total of 50% (50/100) of the families who complete the study protocol will be invited to participate in a second wave of data collection 3 to 12 months after their initial enrollment. The 9-month discrepancy in the timing of the second wave is due to rolling recruitment. The purpose of the second wave of data collection is to evaluate the feasibility of retaining the sample over time and to assess the longer-term acceptability of the protocol. The protocol will be deemed feasible and acceptable if 80% of those invited agree to re-enroll in wave 2 of the study. Furthermore, the wave 2 protocol will be adapted based on wave 1 participant feedback.

Statistical Analyses

Data will be analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows version 27. The descriptive statistics of the feasibility outcomes will be presented. As feasibility is the primary outcome of interest, we will use standard effect size estimates (ie, Cohen d and r) and minimal acceptable feasibility metrics in favor of significance testing [66]. Two-tailed independent sample t tests will be used to examine the differences between those who withdraw from the study and those who complete the protocol.

Sample Size

A sample size of 100 is adequate to evaluate the feasibility of the multibehavior protocol to measure digital media use and screen time context among preschoolers and their families, allowing for generalization to other families with children of similar age and demographics. Given that this is a pilot study, no power analysis is required [67].

Ethics Approval

The study protocol was approved by the institutional review board of the University of South Carolina (Approval Number: Pro00092634).


The Tots and Tech study was funded in March 2020. Data collection began in September 2020 and was completed in May 2022. We aim to enroll 100 caregiver-child dyads. The Tots and Tech outcome paper is expected to be published in 2022.


Overview

Excessive screen time has been linked to poor physical health outcomes (ie, obesity) [68] and mental health outcomes (ie, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and externalizing behaviors) [69,70]. However, studies evaluating the relationship between screen time and health behaviors have significant limitations. The existing studies on screen time have focused on television viewing, largely neglected digital media use [71], relied on retrospective parental reports of average screen time [12], and not yet examined individual variability or temporality [72] of screen time within a day. This study protocol is designed to address these weaknesses by (1) leveraging data already collected by smartphones and tablets (ie, app use) to objectively measure smartphone screen time and (2) integrating intensive longitudinal data collected from multiple sources (ie, passive mobile screen time sensing, EMA, and accelerometry).

This protocol attempts to overcome the methodological limitations of previous studies by incorporating objective measures of mobile screen time and health behaviors (physical activity, sedentary time, and sleep) in conjunction with EMA. The intention of this protocol is to measure screen time behaviors among caregivers and their preschool-age children more accurately. The results will be used to evaluate the feasibility and utility of a comprehensive multibehavior protocol to measure digital media use and screen time context. If this protocol is deemed feasible and acceptable, it can be used in future observational and intervention studies to better measure screen time and its context. In addition, this protocol makes it possible to examine more complex predictors of child health behaviors, such as the within-dyad daily dynamics and between-dyad associations between parenting practices and child behavior problems.

However, our methodology is not without limitations. Objectively monitoring mobile devices (eg, smartphones and tablets) can only provide objective information on digital media use, which is only one of the sources of screen time. It is likely that children and their caregivers also use other types of screen media (ie, television viewing, computers, and shared tablets). This additional screen use is assessed using a combination of EMA measures and traditional questionnaire data. Although the use of parent-report measures of children’s additional screen time is less than ideal, the EMA method of intermittent assessment could reduce the recall bias present in many existing measures of typical screen use. Using this EMA method of screen assessment can provide temporal context to examine the within-day variability of screen time patterns. Future studies could leverage the emerging advances in facial recognition and machine-learning technology to passively and objectively measure additional forms of screen use among families.

The current methods of smartphone-based digital media–use assessment are also limited by the inability to determine who is using the smartphone. We attempt to disentangle smartphone use by determining whether the apps used are designed for children or adults. During qualitative interviews, caregivers indicate whether frequently used apps are used by themselves or by their children and provide information about who has access to the smartphone during peak time windows.

For the purpose of this feasibility study, all device use is considered screen time. However, the protocol for monitoring screen time on Android (but not iOS) devices provides information on how the device is being used (eg, app use) at specific times throughout the measurement period. Future studies can use this protocol to evaluate the differential effects of different types of screen time. For example, do children exhibit more behavioral problems following playing games on their tablet compared to video chatting? These studies are needed to advance science and inform guidelines for children’s screen time.

To avoid overlapping time points and minimize missing data due to noncompliance [43], participants are sent 4 EMAs per day; these EMAs do not assess every hour of the day and, therefore, likely miss important screen time context and resultant child behaviors. Nevertheless, the methodology used by the Tots and Tech study greatly improves upon common methods in child screen time literature.

Conclusions

This study is designed to evaluate the feasibility and utility of a comprehensive multibehavior protocol to measure digital media use and screen time context among a racially and economically diverse sample of preschoolers and their families. The findings will inform protocol adjustments in preparation for a future well-powered study. Ultimately, the Tots and Tech study aims to reveal the process-oriented science that underlies the association between screen time and physical (ie, sleep and activity) and mental health (ie, behavioral problems). Preliminary data from this study will inform model convergence statistics and provide informative priors for the intraclass correlation coefficient and path estimates. These are necessary to conduct a power analysis to inform a future well-powered observational cohort study to examine these connections in further detail over time. The study protocol uses EMAs and objectively measured methods that can reveal the temporal mechanisms of health behavior. Understanding individual-level behavioral patterns has the potential to advance the science of personalized intervention approaches and inform health behavior theories to improve the health and well-being of children and their families.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health P20GM130420 for their support.

Data Availability

Data sharing is not applicable to this study, as no data sets were generated or analyzed during the study.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

Multimedia Appendix 1

Tots and Tech qualitative interview guide.

DOCX File , 26 KB

  1. Tomopoulos S, Dreyer BP, Valdez P, Flynn V, Foley G, Berkule SB, et al. Media content and externalizing behaviors in Latino toddlers. Ambul Pediatr 2007;7(3):232-238. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  2. Hinkley T, Verbestel V, Ahrens W, Lissner L, Molnár D, Moreno LA, IDEFICS Consortium. Early childhood electronic media use as a predictor of poorer well-being: a prospective cohort study. JAMA Pediatr 2014 May;168(5):485-492. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  3. Stiglic N, Viner RM. Effects of screentime on the health and well-being of children and adolescents: a systematic review of reviews. BMJ Open 2019 Jan 03;9(1):e023191 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  4. Carter B, Rees P, Hale L, Bhattacharjee D, Paradkar MS. Association between portable screen-based media device access or use and sleep outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr 2016 Dec 01;170(12):1202-1208 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for children under 5 years of age. World Health Organization. 2019.   URL: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/311664 [accessed 2021-11-19]
  6. Chaput JP, Colley RC, Aubert S, Carson V, Janssen I, Roberts KC, et al. Proportion of preschool-aged children meeting the Canadian 24-Hour Movement Guidelines and associations with adiposity: results from the Canadian Health Measures Survey. BMC Public Health 2017 Nov 20;17(Suppl 5):829 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  7. Rideout V, Robb MB. The Common Sense census: media use by kids age zero to eight. Common Sense Media. 2020.   URL: https:/​/www.​commonsensemedia.org/​sites/​default/​files/​research/​report/​2020_zero_to_eight_census_final_web.​pdf [accessed 2021-11-19]
  8. Yuan N, Weeks HM, Ball R, Newman MW, Chang YJ, Radesky JS. How much do parents actually use their smartphones? Pilot study comparing self-report to passive sensing. Pediatr Res 2019 Oct;86(4):416-418 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  9. Robinson JL, Winiewicz DD, Fuerch JH, Roemmich JN, Epstein LH. Relationship between parental estimate and an objective measure of child television watching. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2006 Nov 27;3:43 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Parry DA, Davidson BI, Sewall CJ, Fisher JT, Mieczkowski H, Quintana DS. A systematic review and meta-analysis of discrepancies between logged and self-reported digital media use. Nat Hum Behav 2021 Nov;5(11):1535-1547. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  11. Bryant MJ, Lucove JC, Evenson KR, Marshall S. Measurement of television viewing in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Obes Rev 2007 May;8(3):197-209. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  12. Paudel S, Jancey J, Subedi N, Leavy J. Correlates of mobile screen media use among children aged 0-8: a systematic review. BMJ Open 2017 Oct 24;7(10):e014585 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  13. Byrne R, Terranova CO, Trost SG. Measurement of screen time among young children aged 0-6 years: a systematic review. Obes Rev 2021 Aug;22(8):e13260 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  14. Linebarger DN, Barr R. Conclusions: making screens make sense for young children. In: Barr R, Linebarger DN, editors. Media Exposure During Infancy and Early Childhood: The Effects of Content and Context on Learning and Development. Berlin, Germany: Springer International Publishing; 2017:291-296.
  15. Vandewater EA, Lee SJ. Measuring children's media use in the digital age: issues and challenges. Am Behav Sci 2009 Apr 01;52(8):1152-1176 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  16. Radesky JS, Weeks HM, Ball R, Schaller A, Yeo S, Durnez J, et al. Young children's use of smartphones and tablets. Pediatrics 2020 Jul;146(1):e20193518 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  17. Ryding FC, Kuss DJ. Passive objective measures in the assessment of problematic smartphone use: a systematic review. Addict Behav Rep 2020 Jun;11:100257 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  18. Barr R, Kirkorian H, Radesky J, Coyne S, Nichols D, Blanchfield O, et al. Beyond screen time: a synergistic approach to a more comprehensive assessment of family media exposure during early childhood. Front Psychol 2020 Jul 10;11:1283 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  19. Armstrong B, Covington LB, Hager ER, Black MM. Objective sleep and physical activity using 24-hour ankle-worn accelerometry among toddlers from low-income families. Sleep Health 2019 Oct;5(5):459-465 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  20. Trofholz A, Tate A, Janowiec M, Fertig A, Loth K, de Brito JN, et al. Ecological momentary assessment of weight-related behaviors in the home environment of children from low-income and racially and ethnically diverse households: development and usability study. JMIR Res Protoc 2021 Dec 01;10(12):e30525 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  21. Jones A, Armstrong B, Weaver RG, Parker H, von Klinggraeff L, Beets MW. Identifying effective intervention strategies to reduce children's screen time: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2021 Sep 16;18(1):126 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  22. Maclure M, Mittleman MA. Should we use a case-crossover design? Annu Rev Public Health 2000;21:193-221. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  23. Turner-McGrievy G, Halliday TM, Moore JB. COVID-19 messed up my research: insights from physical activity and nutrition translational research. Transl J Am Coll Sports Med 2021;6(4):e000169. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. Poverty Thresholds. United States Census Bureau.   URL: https:/​/www.​census.gov/​data/​tables/​time-series/​demo/​income-poverty/​historical-poverty-thresholds.​html [accessed 2021-11-18]
  25. Bickel G, Nord M, Price C, Hamilton W, Cook J. Guide to measuring household food security. United States Department of Agriculture. 2000.   URL: https://naldc.nal.usda.gov/download/38369/PDF [accessed 2021-11-19]
  26. Hager ER, Quigg AM, Black MM, Coleman SM, Heeren T, Rose-Jacobs R, et al. Development and validity of a 2-item screen to identify families at risk for food insecurity. Pediatrics 2010 Jul;126(1):e26-e32. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  27. Isong IA, Richmond T, Kawachi I, Avendaño M. Childcare attendance and obesity risk. Pediatrics 2016 Nov;138(5):e20161539 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  28. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav 1983 Dec;24(4):385-396. [Medline]
  29. Matheny Jr AP, Wachs TD, Ludwig JL, Phillips K. Bringing order out of chaos: psychometric characteristics of the confusion, hubbub, and order scale. J Appl Dev Psychol 1995 Jul;16(3):429-444. [CrossRef]
  30. Andresen EM, Malmgren JA, Carter WB, Patrick DL. Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale). Am J Prev Med 1994;10(2):77-84. [Medline]
  31. Marteau TM, Bekker H. The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Br J Clin Psychol 1992 Sep;31(3):301-306. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  32. Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001 Nov;40(11):1337-1345. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  33. James DE, Schumm WR, Kennedy CE, Grigsby CC, Shectman KL, Nichols CW. Characteristics of the Kansas parental satisfaction scale among two samples of married parents. Psychol Rep 1985 Aug;57(1):163-169. [CrossRef]
  34. Alfano CA, Smith VC, Reynolds KC, Reddy R, Dougherty LR. The Parent-Child Sleep Interactions Scale (PSIS) for preschoolers: factor structure and initial psychometric properties. J Clin Sleep Med 2013 Nov 15;9(11):1153-1160 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  35. Snyder E, Cai B, DeMuro C, Morrison MF, Ball W. A new single-item sleep quality scale: results of psychometric evaluation in patients with chronic primary insomnia and depression. J Clin Sleep Med 2018 Nov 15;14(11):1849-1857 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  36. Turco M, Corrias M, Chiaromanni F, Bano M, Salamanca M, Caccin L, et al. The self-morningness/eveningness (Self-ME): an extremely concise and totally subjective assessment of diurnal preference. Chronobiol Int 2015;32(9):1192-1200. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  37. Vizcaino M, Buman M, DesRoches CT, Wharton C. Reliability of a new measure to assess modern screen time in adults. BMC Public Health 2019 Oct 28;19(1):1386 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  38. Cingel DP, Krcmar M. Predicting media use in very young children: the role of demographics and parent attitudes. Commun Stud 2013 Sep;64(4):374-394. [CrossRef]
  39. Sanders W, Parent J, Forehand R, Breslend NL. The roles of general and technology-related parenting in managing youth screen time. J Fam Psychol 2016 Aug;30(5):641-646 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  40. Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS, Grummer-Strawn LM, Flegal KM, Mei Z, et al. 2000 CDC growth charts for the United States: methods and development. Vital Health Stat 11 2002 May(246):1-190 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  41. Li JJ, Lansford JE. A smartphone-based ecological momentary assessment of parental behavioral consistency: associations with parental stress and child ADHD symptoms. Dev Psychol 2018 Jun;54(6):1086-1098 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  42. Wakschlag LS, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Choi SW, Nichols SR, Kestler J, Burns JL, et al. Advancing a multidimensional, developmental spectrum approach to preschool disruptive behavior. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2014 Jan;53(1):82-96.e3 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  43. Dunton GF, Liao Y, Dzubur E, Leventhal AM, Huh J, Gruenewald T, et al. Investigating within-day and longitudinal effects of maternal stress on children's physical activity, dietary intake, and body composition: protocol for the MATCH study. Contemp Clin Trials 2015 Jul;43:142-154 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Berge JM, Tate A, Trofholz A, Fertig AR, Miner M, Crow S, et al. Momentary parental stress and food-related parenting practices. Pediatrics 2017 Dec;140(6):e20172295 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  45. O'Connor SG, Habre R, Bastain TM, Toledo-Corral CM, Gilliland FD, Eckel SP, et al. Within-subject effects of environmental and social stressors on pre- and post-partum obesity-related biobehavioral responses in low-income Hispanic women: protocol of an intensive longitudinal study. BMC Public Health 2019 Feb 28;19(1):253 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  46. Mason TB, O'Connor SG, Schembre SM, Huh J, Chu D, Dunton GF. Momentary affect, stress coping, and food intake in mother-child dyads. Health Psychol 2019 Mar;38(3):238-247 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  47. Leppert KA, Bufferd SJ, Olino TM, Dougherty LR. A daily diary analysis of preschool depressive behaviors: prospective associations and moderators across 14 days. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2019 Sep;47(9):1547-1558 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  48. Burke LE, Shiffman S, Music E, Styn MA, Kriska A, Smailagic A, et al. Ecological momentary assessment in behavioral research: addressing technological and human participant challenges. J Med Internet Res 2017 Mar 15;19(3):e77 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  49. Degroote L, DeSmet A, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Van Dyck D, Crombez G. Content validity and methodological considerations in ecological momentary assessment studies on physical activity and sedentary behaviour: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2020 Mar 10;17(1):35 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  50. Bolger N, DeLongis A, Kessler RC, Schilling EA. Effects of daily stress on negative mood. J Pers Soc Psychol 1989 Nov;57(5):808-818. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  51. Bai Y, Chen S, Laurson KR, Kim Y, Saint-Maurice PF, Welk GJ. The associations of youth physical activity and screen time with fatness and fitness: the 2012 NHANES National Youth Fitness Survey. PLoS One 2016 Jan 28;11(1):e0148038 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  52. Shiffman S. Designing protocols for ecological momentary assessment. In: Stone A, Shiffman S, Atienza A, Nebeling L, editors. The Science of Real-Time Data Capture: Self-reports in health research. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2007:27-53.
  53. Munsch S, Meyer AH, Milenkovic N, Schlup B, Margraf J, Wilhelm FH. Ecological momentary assessment to evaluate cognitive-behavioral treatment for binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord 2009 Nov;42(7):648-657. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  54. Liao Y, Skelton K, Dunton G, Bruening M. A systematic review of methods and procedures used in ecological momentary assessments of diet and physical activity research in youth: an adapted STROBE checklist for reporting EMA studies (CREMAS). J Med Internet Res 2016 Jun 21;18(6):e151 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  55. Fairclough SJ, Noonan R, Rowlands AV, Van Hees V, Knowles Z, Boddy LM. Wear compliance and activity in children wearing wrist- and hip-mounted accelerometers. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2016 Feb;48(2):245-253. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  56. Migueles JH, Rowlands AV, Huber F, Sabia S, van Hees VT. GGIR: a research community–driven open source r package for generating physical activity and sleep outcomes from multi-day raw accelerometer data. J Meas Phys Behav 2019 Sep 23;2(3):188-196. [CrossRef]
  57. van Hees VT, Gorzelniak L, Dean León EC, Eder M, Pias M, Taherian S, et al. Separating movement and gravity components in an acceleration signal and implications for the assessment of human daily physical activity. PLoS One 2013 Apr 23;8(4):e61691 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  58. Roscoe CM, James RS, Duncan MJ. Calibration of GENEActiv accelerometer wrist cut-points for the assessment of physical activity intensity of preschool aged children. Eur J Pediatr 2017 Aug;176(8):1093-1098. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  59. Hildebrand M, Hansen BH, van Hees VT, Ekelund U. Evaluation of raw acceleration sedentary thresholds in children and adults. Scand J Med Sci Sports 2017 Dec;27(12):1814-1823. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  60. Hildebrand M, VAN Hees VT, Hansen BH, Ekelund U. Age group comparability of raw accelerometer output from wrist- and hip-worn monitors. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2014 Sep;46(9):1816-1824. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  61. van Hees VT, Sabia S, Anderson KN, Denton SJ, Oliver J, Catt M, et al. A novel, open access method to assess sleep duration using a wrist-worn accelerometer. PLoS One 2015 Nov 16;10(11):e0142533 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  62. Sinnott C, Byrne M, Bradley CP. Improving medication management for patients with multimorbidity in primary care: a qualitative feasibility study of the MY COMRADE implementation intervention. Pilot Feasibility Stud 2017 Mar 20;3:14 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  63. Ramsey AT, Wetherell JL, Depp C, Dixon D, Lenze E. Feasibility and acceptability of smartphone assessment in older adults with cognitive and emotional difficulties. J Technol Hum Serv 2016;34(2):209-223 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  64. Swendeman D, Sumstine S, Brink A, Mindry D, Medich M, Russell M. Smartphone self-monitoring by young adolescents and parents to assess and improve family functioning: qualitative feasibility study. JMIR Form Res 2020 Jun 23;4(6):e15777 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  65. Moitra E, Gaudiano BA, Davis CH, Ben-Zeev D. Feasibility and acceptability of post-hospitalization ecological momentary assessment in patients with psychotic-spectrum disorders. Compr Psychiatry 2017 Apr;74:204-213 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  66. Orsmond GI, Cohn ES. The distinctive features of a feasibility study: objectives and guiding questions. OTJR (Thorofare N J) 2015 Jul;35(3):169-177. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  67. Eldridge SM, Chan CL, Campbell MJ, Bond CM, Hopewell S, Thabane L, PAFS consensus group. CONSORT 2010 statement: extension to randomised pilot and feasibility trials. BMJ 2016 Oct 24;355:i5239 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  68. Peck T, Scharf RJ, Conaway MR, DeBoer MD. Viewing as little as 1 hour of TV daily is associated with higher change in BMI between kindergarten and first grade. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2015 Aug;23(8):1680-1686 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  69. McNeill J, Howard SJ, Vella SA, Cliff DP. Longitudinal associations of electronic application use and media program viewing with cognitive and psychosocial development in preschoolers. Acad Pediatr 2019 Jul;19(5):520-528. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  70. Nikkelen SW, Valkenburg PM, Huizinga M, Bushman BJ. Media use and ADHD-related behaviors in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Dev Psychol 2014 Sep;50(9):2228-2241. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  71. Thomas G, Bennie JA, De Cocker K, Castro O, Biddle SJ. A descriptive epidemiology of screen-based devices by children and adolescents: a scoping review of 130 surveillance studies since 2000. Child Ind Res 2020;13(3):935-950. [CrossRef]
  72. Hale L, Guan S. Screen time and sleep among school-aged children and adolescents: a systematic literature review. Sleep Med Rev 2015 Jun;21:50-58 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]


EMA: ecological momentary assessment


Edited by T Leung; submitted 07.01.22; peer-reviewed by J Maher, MM Patwary; comments to author 25.07.22; revised version received 10.08.22; accepted 29.08.22; published 28.09.22

Copyright

©Layton Reesor-Oyer, Hannah Parker, Sarah Burkart, Michal T Smith, Roddrick Dugger, Lauren von Klinggraeff, R Glenn Weaver, Michael W Beets, Bridget Armstrong. Originally published in JMIR Research Protocols (https://www.researchprotocols.org), 28.09.2022.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Research Protocols, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://www.researchprotocols.org, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.